BU Today

Science & Tech

The Dyslexia Paradox

SAR prof, colleagues may have pinpointed a root of reading disorder

1
Tyler Perrachione

Tyler Perrachione looks for the source of reading disorders, like dyslexia, in the brain. Photo by Jackie Ricciardi

It’s there, at the start of every conversation: the moment it takes your brain to adjust to an unfamiliar voice. It lasts for only a second or two, but in that brief time, your brain is thumbing its radio dial, tuning in to the unique pitch, rhythm, accent, and vowel sounds of a new voice. Once it is dialed in, the conversation can take off.

This process is called rapid neural adaptation, and it happens constantly. New voices, sounds, sights, feelings, tastes, and smells all trigger this brain response. It is so effortless that we are rarely even aware it’s happening. But, according to new work from Tyler Perrachione, a Sargent College of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences assistant professor of speech, language, and hearing sciences, and colleagues at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and Massachusetts General Hospital, problems with neural adaptation may be at the root of dyslexia, a reading impairment that affects millions of Americans, including an estimated one in 5 to one in 20 schoolchildren. Their experiments, published December 21, 2016, in the journal Neuron, are the first to use brain imaging to compare neural adaptation in the brains of people with dyslexia and those who read normally. The research was supported by the Lawrence Ellison Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, and the National Science Foundation.

In the team’s first experiment, volunteers without dyslexia were asked to pair spoken words with images on a screen while the researchers used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to track their brain activity. The subjects tried the test two different ways. In one version, they listened to words spoken by a variety of different voices. In the second version, they heard the words all spoken by the same voice. As the researchers expected, the fMRI revealed an initial spike of activity in the brain’s language network at the start of both tests. But during the first test, the brain continued revving with each new word and voice. When the voice stayed the same in the second test, the brain did not have to work as hard. It adapted.

But when subjects with dyslexia took the same tests, their brain activity never eased off. Like a radio that can’t hold a frequency, the brain did not adapt to the consistent voice and had to process it fresh every time, as if it were new. The difference was even clearer in dyslexic children between ages six and nine, who were just learning to read; in a similar experiment, their brains didn’t adapt at all to repeated words.

fMRI images show how people with dyslexia (right) and people without (left) adapt differently to a speaker’s voice

These fMRI images show how people with dyslexia (right) and people without (left) adapt differently to a speaker’s voice. The colored regions show adaptation, or the change in brain activation upon hearing a voice for the first time, and hearing it repeatedly. The average of nondyslexic brains shows stronger adaptation than the average of dyslexic brains. Courtesy of Tyler Perrachione

Perrachione and his colleagues wondered if the adaptation glitch was unique to spoken words, or if people with dyslexia would have trouble adapting to other kinds of stimuli, too. So they tried a second set of experiments, showing subjects a repeating series of words, pictures, or faces, again using fMRI to look for the decline in brain activity that signals neural adaptation. Again, they found that the brains of people with dyslexia did not adapt—or did not adapt as well—as those without. “We found the signature everywhere we looked,” says Perrachione.

The results suggest that dyslexic brains have to work harder than “typical” brains to process incoming sights and sounds, requiring additional mental overhead for even the simplest tasks. “What was surprising for me was the magnitude of the difference. These are not subtle differences,” says Perrachione. This finding dovetails with his other work on the dyslexic brain, which has found that individuals with dyslexia also struggle with phonological working memory. The extra brainwork might not be noticeable most of the time, but it seems to have a singularly prominent impact on reading.

The results could solve a paradox that has stumped dyslexia researchers for decades. “People with dyslexia have a specific problem with reading, yet there is no ‘reading part’ of our brain,” says Neuron article coauthor John Gabrieli, an MIT professor of brain and cognitive sciences, who was Perrachione’s PhD advisor when he conducted much of the research reported in the paper. Injuries to specific parts of the brain can cause people to lose particular skills, like the ability to speak, that sit in those brain regions. But because the brain doesn’t have a discrete reading center, it’s hard to understand how a disorder could handicap reading and only reading.

This new work partially solves the paradox because rapid neural adaptation is a “low-level” function of the brain, which acts as a building block for “higher-level,” abstract functions. Yet that opens up another mystery, says Gabrieli. “Why are there other domains that are so well done by people with reading difficulty?”

Image of black dots of a dalmation

This optical illusion shows rapid neural adaptation in action. At first, the image looks like random black spots. But look a little longer and you will see a Dalmatian. (The dog’s nose is in the center of the image and his body extends to the right.) After seeing the Dalmatian once, most people see it every time they view the picture, even if they don’t explicitly remember where to look. Neural adaptation has primed the brain to read the spots in a new way. Photo by Ronald C. James

The answer has to do with the way we learn to read, the researchers think: “There’s almost nothing we learn that’s as complicated as reading.” That’s because learning to read is mentally cumbersome. The human brain did not evolve to read—literacy has been commonplace only in the last two centuries—so the brain must repurpose regions that evolved for very different ends. And the evolutionary newness of reading may leave the brain without a backup plan. “Reading is so demanding that there’s not a successful alternative pathway that works as well,” says Gabrieli. It’s like using a stapler to pound a nail—the stapler can get the job done, but it takes a lot of extra effort.

The fMRI results show which parts of the brain are straining but don’t tell researchers exactly why people with dyslexia have a different adaptation response. In the future, Perrachione and his colleagues hope to examine how neurons and neurotransmitters change during adaptation. “Finding a basic thing that’s true in the whole brain gives us a better opportunity to start looking for connections between biological models and psychological models,” says Perrachione. Those connections may one day lead to better ways to identify and treat children with dyslexia.

1 Comments

One Comment on The Dyslexia Paradox

  • Virginia on 02.03.2017 at 3:05 pm

    As a 53 year old dyslexic I am fascinated by these results. As a child I could really feel my brain almost short circuiting when working with letters or numbers, but give me puzzle and I could put those together with no effort. To this day I am a better visual learner than an audio learner. I remember being in a summer reading class in the hopes I would not have to repeat 3rd grade and literally one day realizing that all those letters on the page made sense ALL of the time! By the end of the summer I went from not reading at all to reading on a 6th grade level. For me it was and is a miracle. When I was tested and diagnosed with dyslexia my parents were told by the testers that they did not know how I unraveled the English language, but I found my own way. I have two other friends who are close to my age and we all have great spatial relationship abilities and navigational skills. We always know whether we are heading north, south, east or west. I wonder why this is the case? Are our brains compensating for being deficient in other areas? From and evolutionary point of view, if we had problems listening to our fellow cavemen when hunting for woolly mammoths and following directions so we didn’t get trampled did we develop other skills so we could stay in the gene pool?

Post Your Comment

(never shown)