With a flip of a switch, Tania Singer triggered an electrical
shock to her female subject’s right hand. With a flip of another switch,
the subject’s husband received a similar shock. The woman, seated next
to an MRI scanner, could see nothing of her husband except for his hand, but
she watched a screen indicating whether she or her partner was receiving a painless
electrical shock or one about as painful as a bee sting. The MRIs showed that
when witnessing her loved one in pain, certain parts of the woman’s brain
would become active, echoing a similar brain response when she herself was in
pain. This example of empathy showed that, in a limited sense, the phrase “I
feel your pain,” can be true.
Empathy – the act of knowing how others feel, feeling what others feel
and caring what others feel – has long been the domain of philosophers
and social scientists. This study, published in Science in early 2004, marks
the most recent example of a move in empathy research from the field of social
science to neurology. Empathy is the English translation of the German word
Einfuhlung, which literally means “feeling into.” The idea was first
presented in 1873 by German theorist Robert Vischer as a term used in aesthetics
– the theory that dynamics in a work of art could be used to suggest muscular
and emotional attitudes in a viewer, making them experience the related emotions.
It wasn’t until 1903 that empathy entered the field of psychology thanks
to philosopher Theodore Lipps. He put forth the theory that perceiving an emotion
in another person can activate the same emotion in the viewer.
Over a century later, Tania Singer, a research fellow at the University College
of London, has the MRI scans that prove him right.
To study empathy, Singer needed to differentiate between a patient feeling an
emotion and a patient witnessing it in someone else, to determine if the same
neural circuits were involved. Scientists are finding that it’s possible
to be empathetic without fully taking on the feelings involved. Though the brain
does echo what would occur if the feeling or stimulus was applied to the self,
it is not a true mirror image when it is applied to others.
Singer and her colleagues examined how much of another person’s pain their
subjects would feel – that is, how empathetic they were to the pain. They
chose 16 couples because they assumed that couples would be more likely to be
empathetic towards each other, said Singer. The women were seated next to MRI
scanners that were able to measure the brain activity by looking at the blood
flow to certain regions of the brain. As certain regions of the brain are activated,
blood flow increases and they light up on the scanner. By comparing the MRI
images of a woman when she was being shocked to when her husband was being shocked,
the scientists were able to see what areas of the brain were triggered by the
physical sensation of pain versus the knowledge that a loved one was feeling
that pain.
Singer and her colleagues found that the women responded to their partner’s
pain by echoing some of the same brain activity that would occur when they themselves
were in pain, but not all of it. They only felt some of their partner’s
pain. The women’s brains failed to register the partner’s pain as
a physical sensation of pain, but rather evoked the emotional suffering of what
that pain would feel like.
What actually occurs when the brain feels empathy is a complex behavior that
relies on several systems, says Marco Iacoboni, a neuroscientist at the University
of California, Los Angeles. One of the key participants is an almond-shaped
region of the forebrain known as the amygdala, which showed increased blood
flow. This makes sense, since the amygdala is part of the limbic system or “emotional
brain.” The region has strong connections to people’s emotions,
especially fear and nervous reactions, and scientists suspect it mediates both
inborn and acquired emotional responses as well. Now scientists are finding
the frontal lobe is also activated during the studies, making it a key component
of empathy.
“Although empathy is experienced as a feeling, it has a cognitive as well
as affective component,” according to William Damon, a developmental psychologist
at Stanford University. The frontal lobe is part of the cerebrum, the more cognitive
part of the brain. It receives information from the senses and the emotions
and integrates that data into a plan of action for the individual, as well as
choosing whether or not to follow through with the plan. So empathy employs
not only the feeling part of the brain, but thinking parts as well.
As a fundamental building block of our social interactions, empathy plays a
vital role in how we interact with each other. “Empathy puts the brakes
on violence. Empathy allows communication. Empathy allows one to understand
another’s behavior. Empathy allows altruism,” says Simon Baron-Cohen,
a research fellow at the University of Cambridge. “Do you need any more
reasons to justify the importance of empathy?”
The ability to put oneself in another’s position hinges on the theory
of mind – the ability to attribute mental states like our own to others,
taking into account what others are thinking. This usually develops around age
four, allowing people to understand that other people are like themselves, with
their own internal mental lives, and makes it possible to not only learn from
each other, but through each other’s experience. “There is good
evidence that we can figure out how others are feeling, what they intend and
how they are likely to act by putting ourselves in their shoes, so to speak,”
said Ralph Adolphs, a neurologist at the University of Iowa.
Putting ourselves in another’s shoes involves a certain amount of imitation,
whether it’s an outer expression of the emotion like facial expressions
and posture or an inner expression through the activation of similar regions
of the brain. In a study on imitation, Iacoboni found that overt imitation of
gestures or facial expressions increased the inner level of imitation; increased
the amount of brain activity that was echoed. Eleven subjects were underwent
MRI scanning while they either imitated or simply observed various facial expressions
from happiness and surprise to anger, sadness, disgust and fear. When the subjects
simply observed the faces, activity occurred in the emotional centers of the
brain, like the amygdala. When the subjects actually imitated the faces, the
brain became far more active. Part of the activity was in regions that deal
with physical motion, which wouldn’t be triggered by observation alone,
but even the areas like the amygdala that only deal with emotion showed more
blood flow, meaning more activity. “The more one tends to imitate others,”
said Iacobini, “the more one is empathetic.”
The imaging data corroborates previous work that used the older psychological
questionnaires. Through the surveys, scientists found that people with something
called the chameleon effect – a tendency to imitate mannerisms, expressions,
accents and postures of other people automatically and subconsciously –
would test higher on the empathy scales. The more in tune their physical body
was to the people around them, the more in tune they were to the mental aspects
of those surrounding them. “If you really want to understand what the
mental state of the person is, you still have to invoke some of the mental aspects
that are associated with that state,” said Iacoboni.
The use of brain imaging in empathy has led researchers to consider empathy
as more than just a philosophical moral value, but as something that is actually
hard-wired into the brain. Scientists not only see how empathy is expressed
neurologically, but are able to find out the explanations for the information
they’ve learned from psychological empathy testing. For instance, it’s
long been believed, based on survey evaluations, that women are more empathetic
than men. According to Baron-Cohen, women average about ten points higher than
men on EQ scores, the emotional equivalent of an IQ test. That’s why Singer’s
pain study focused on women. “They are known for being more empathetic
but there is no brain scan evidence of it,” she said.
But there may soon be. Singer’s latest work is will switch the roles between
the men and women studied to see if men show a similar empathic response. Baron-Cohen
is wrapping up a study that uses MRIs to show the difference in empathetic response
between men and women. They hope that through brain imaging, they will answer
the question of whether there is an actual biological difference in both how
and to what extent men and women feel empathy.